Acute stress impairs error monitoring and post-error adjustment

HU Na1 CHEN Antao1 WANG Yanqing2,3 LI Qing1 XU Zhenzhen1 LONG Quanshan1

(1.Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715)
(2.Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101)
(3.Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049)

【Abstract】Stressor presents a risk factor in everyday life by not only triggering stress responses in the body but also influencing cognitive processing. Previous research has shown that the medial frontal cortex and dorsolateral frontal cortex, on which error processing depends, are susceptible to acute stress. However, few studies have explored the effect of stress on error processing. It is still unclear whether individuals with acute stress can effectively detect their own error responses and how acute stress influences the transfer from error monitoring to post-error adjustment. To address these issues, we recruited 52 healthy male participants and randomly assigned them into stress (n = 26) or control (n = 26) groups. The participants were first asked to undergo an acute stress test or control-stress test and soon after that perform an error awareness task. Acute stress was induced by the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) which consists of a public speech task and a mental arithmetic task. The error awareness task was a motor go/no-go response inhibition task, in which the participants marked the responses where they recognized errors. Additionally, information on the participants’ heart rates, subjective emotional states, and perceived stress levels was collected to evaluate their immediate reaction to stress. Finally, we used salivary cortisol levels to explore the delayed reaction to stress during the experiment. The acute stress induction was indexed by the increases in free cortisol levels, heart rates, perceived stress levels, and negative affect in the stress group compared with the control group. Two main findings were obtained in this study. First, the accuracy of error awareness in the stress group was lower than that of the control group, and the negative affect under acute stress was negatively predictive of the accuracy of error awareness in the stress group but not the control group, suggesting that acute stress led to poor error monitoring. Second, the accuracy of the first trials after aware errors was significantly lower than that after unaware errors in the stress group, but there was no change in the control group, showing that post-error performance was impaired following acute stress. Altogether, the present study demonstrated that participants could not effectively identify error responses after acute stress, and their post-error adjustment was impaired even when the error responses were identified. Our results show that stress plays an important role in error processing, which is consistent with the biphasic-reciprocal model that proposes that stress responses enhance the activation of the amygdala but impair neural functioning in the prefrontal cortex. In conclusion, the present study demonstrates that acute stress impairs the performance monitoring system, which leads to impaired post-error adaptive behaviors.

【Keywords】 acute stress; error awareness; cortisol; post-error adjustment;

【DOI】

【Funds】 National Natural Science Foundation of China (61431013, 31771254) Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (SWU1609106, SWU1709107, SWU1809361)

Download this article

    References

    Arnsten, A. F. (2015). Stress weakens prefrontal networks: Molecular insults to higher cognition. Nature Neuroscience, 18(10), 1376–1385.

    Banich, M. T. (2009). Executive function: The search for an integrated account. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 89–94.

    Beck, A. T. (1968). Depression: Clinical, experimental and theoretical aspects. JAMA, 203(13), 1144–1145.

    Bogdanov, M., & Schwabe, L. (2016). Transcranial stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex prevents stress-induced working memory deficits. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(4), 1429–1437.

    Botvinick, M. M., Bra ver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652.

    Buzzell, G. A., Beatty, P. J., Paquette, N. A., Roberts, D. M., & McDonald, C. G. (2017). Error-induced blindness: Error detection leads to impaired sensory processing and lower accuracy at short response-stimulus intervals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(11), 2895–2903.

    Cavanagh, J. F., & Allen, J. J. (2008). Multiple aspects of the stress response under social evaluative threat: An electrophysiological investigation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(1), 41–53.

    Clemans, Z. A., Elbaz, A. S., Hollifield, M., & Sokhadze, E. M. (2012). Single trial time-frequency domain analysis of error processing in post-traumatic stress disorder. Neuroscience Letters, 525(2), 105–110.

    Danielmeier, C., & Ullsperger, M. (2011). Post-error adjustments. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(233), 233.

    Dedovic, K., Rexroth, M., Wolff, E., Duchesne, A., Scherling, C., Beaudry, T., ... Pruessner, J. C. (2009). Neural correlates of processing stressful information: An event-related fMRIstudy. Brain Research, 1293(1293), 49–60.

    Di Gregorio, F., Steinhauser, M., & Maier, M. E. (2016). Error-related brain activity and error awareness in an error classification paradigm. NeuroImage, 139, 202–210.

    Dickerson, S. S., Gruenewald, T. L., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). When the social self is threatened: Shame, physiology, and health. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1191–1216.

    Dutilh, G., Ravenzwaaij, D. V., Nieuwenhuis, S., Han, L. J. V. D., Forstmann, B. U., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2012). How to measure post-error slowing: A confound and a simple solution. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56(3), 208–216.

    Endrass, T., Reuter, B., & Kathmann, N. (2007). ERP correlates of conscious error recognition: Aware and unaware errors in an antisaccade task. European Journal of Neuroscience, 26(6), 1714–1720.

    Falconer, E., Bryant, R., Felmingham, K. L., Kemp, A. H., Gordon, E., Peduto, A., ... Williams, L. M. (2008). The neural networks of inhibitory control in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 33(5), 413–422.

    Fumi, K., & Christos, C. (2012). Early involvement of prefrontal cortex in visual bottom-up attention. Nature Neuroscience, 15(8), 1160–1166.

    Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (2018). The error-related negativity. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 200–204.

    Hajcak, G., & Dan, F. (2008). Errors are aversive: Defensive motivation and the error-related negativity. Psychological Science, 19(2), 103–108.

    Hermans, E. J., Henckens, M. J., Joëls, M., & Fernández, G. (2014). Dynamic adaptation of large-scale brain networks in response to acute stressors. Trends in neurosciences, 37(6), 304–314.

    Hester, R., Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Shpaner, M., & Garavan, H. (2005). Neural mechanisms involved in error processing: A comparison of errors made with and without awareness. Neuroimage, 27(3), 602–608.

    Hester, R., Simoes-Franklin, C., & Garavan, H. (2007). Post-error behavior in active cocaine users: Poor awareness of errors in the presence of intact performance adjustments. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(9), 1974–1978.

    Hester, R., Nandam, L. S., O'Connell, R. G., Wagner, J., Strudwick, M., Nathan, P. J., ... Nandam, L. S. (20 12). Neurochemical enhancement of conscious error awareness. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(8), 2619–2627.

    Inzlicht, M., Bartholow, B. D., & Hirsh, J. B. (2015). Emotional foundations of cognitive control. Trends in cognitive sciences, 19(3), 126–132.

    Jentzsch, I., & Dudschig, C. (2009). Why do we slow down after an error? Mechanisms underlying the effects of posterror slowing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(2), 209–218.

    Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Gaab, J., Schommer, N. C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1999). Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61(2), 154–162.

    Laming, D. (1979). Choice reaction performance following an error. Acta Psychologica, 43(3), 199–224.

    Laredo, S. A., Steinman, M. Q., Robles, C. F., Ferrer, E., Ragen, B. J., ... Trainor, B. C. (2015). Effects of defeat stress on behavioral flexibility in males and females: Modulation by the mu-opioid receptor. European Journal of Neuroscience, 41(4), 434–441.

    Luu, P., Collins, P., & Tucker, D. M. (2000). Mood, personality, and self-monitoring: Negative affect and emotionality in relation to frontal lobe mechanisms of error monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 129(1), 43–60.

    McReynolds, J. R., Donowho, K., Abdi, A., McGaugh, J. L., Roozendaal, B., & McIntyre, C. K. (2010). Memory enhancing corticosterone treatment increases amygdala norepinephrine and arc protein expression in hippocampal synaptic fractions. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 93(3), 312–321.

    Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 160–166.

    Plessow, F., Fischer, R., Kirschbaum, C., &Goschke, T. (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: Acute psychosocial stress increases shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time lag to the stressor. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3218–3227.

    Pruessner, J. C., Dedovic, K., Khalili-Mahani, N., Engert, V., Pruessner, M., Buss, C., ... Lupien, S. (2008). Deactivation of the limbic system during acute psychosocial stress: Evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biological Psychiatry, 63(2), 234–240.

    Qin, S., Hermans, E. J., van Marle, H. J. F., Luo, J., &Fernández, G. (2009). Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Biological Psychiatry, 66(1), 25–32.

    Roozendaal, B., Okuda, S., van der Zee, E. A., & McGaugh, J. L. (2006). Glucocorticoid enhancement of memory requires arousal-induced noradrenergic activation in the basolateral amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(17), 6741–6746.

    Sänger, J., Bechtold, L., Schoofs, D., Blaszkewicz, M., & Wascher, E. (2014). The influence of acute stress on attention mechanisms and its electrophysiological correlates. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 353.

    Spielberger, C. D. (1989). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Acomprehensive bibliography. Consulting Psychologists Press

    Steinhauser, M., Ernst, B., & Ibald, K. W. (2017). Isolating component processes of posterror slowing with the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(4), 653–659

    Ullsperger, M., & Danielmeier, C. (2016). Reducing speed and sight: How adaptive is post-error slowing? Neuron, 89(3), 430–432.

    Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

    Weinberg, A., Riesel, A., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Integrating multiple perspectives on error-related brain activity: The ERN as a neural indicator of trait defensive reactivity. Motivation & Emotion, 36(1), 84–100.

    Whitton, A. E., Veer, A. V. T., Kakani, P., Dillon, D. G., Ironside, M. L., Haile, A., ... Pizzagalli, D. A. (2017). Acute stress impairs frontocingulate activation du ring error monitoring in remitted depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 75, 164–172.

    Wu, J., Ge, Y., Shi, Z., Duan, X., Wang, L., Sun, X., ... Zhang, K. (2010). Response inhibition in adolescent earthquake survivors with and without posttraumatic stress disorder: Acombined behavioral and ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 486(3), 117–121.

    Wu, J., Yuan, Y., Duan, H., Qin, S., Buchanan, T. W., Zhang, K., ... Liang, Z. (2014). Long-term academic stress increases the late component of error processing: An ERPstudy. Biological psychology, 99, 77–82.

    Vijayraghavan, S., Wang, M., Birnbaum, S. G., Williams, G. V., & Arnsten, A. F. (2007). Inverted-U dopamine D1receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 10(3), 376–384.

    Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 111(4), 931–959.

This Article

ISSN:0439-755X

CN: 11-1911/B

Vol 52, No. 02, Pages 162-172

February 2020

Downloads:0

Share
Article Outline

Knowledge

Abstract

  • 1 Methods
  • 2 Result analysis
  • 3 Discussions
  • 4 Conclusion
  • References