Cross-cultural migration and health: evidence from CLDS data

WANG Ting1 LI Jianmin1

(1.School of Economics, Nankai University, Tianjin, China 300071)

【Abstract】The migration experience is closely related to migrants’ health status, which is related to geographical distance and cultural distance. Cross-cultural migration generally affects migrants’ health through psychological pressure, lifestyle, income and social network. When choosing dialectal, dietetic and conceptual distance as proxy variables of cultural distance, using the data from CLDS, the paper analyzes the cross-cultural migration’s effect to rural migrants’ health as well as the reasons causing this effect. Descriptive statistics show that rural migrants are selective to cultural distance. Most of rural migrants move to the same dialect area and food circle, and the conceptual distance is also close. The Probit model results show that dialectal and conceptual distance has a significantly negative impact on the self-assessed health of rural migrants, and moreover, income level and social network play a role of mediation effect. The influence of dietetic distance is not significant. When applying PSM and IV Probit model to solve endogenous problems, the results reveal that conclusions of this paper are robust. Using two objective indicators for the robustness test, the basic conclusion is still valid. There are heterogeneities caused by the duration of migrants staying in their destination, gender, the number of migrations and local dialect skill when analyzing cultural distance’s effect on rural migrants’ self-assessed health (SAH).

【Keywords】 rural migrants; cross-cultural migration; health;

【DOI】

【Funds】 Youth Project of the National Social Science Fund of China (16CJY013)

Download this article

    Footnote

    [1]. ① The cultural distance index is obtained according to the data of the World Values Survey (WVS), which include the inquiries on the residents of 74 countries and regions around the world concerning their views and attitudes regarding the family, work, life, politics, society, religion and national identity. The index is obtained via calculating the average score of residents of each country/region on the aforementioned questions. The specific method of calculation will be described below. [^Back]

    [2]. ① Migrants refer to the population whose permanent residence is different from the registered residence and who leaves the registered residence for more than half a year. On account of the purpose of this study, this paper excludes the samples of urban-rural migrants, urban-urban migrants, rural residents, and return residents with migrant experiences. [^Back]

    [3]. ① In the CGSS questionnaire, there is the situation that a question is put forward for various attitudes of the respondent. For instance, the question D24 is as follows: do you agree with the following statements, which is treated as multiple questions in this paper. [^Back]

    [4]. ② The equation proposed by Kakwani for measuring the income deprivation of the individual yi is as follows: , where, Y is a group and the sample size is n. Arranged in ascending order of income, y1y2 ≤ . . . ≤ yn, of which the income vector is y = (y1, y2, . . . , yn), with μY as the mean of group Y. [^Back]

    [5]. ① The concept of dialect region is broadened here by involving North China official dialect, Northwest official dialect, Northeast official dialect, Southwest official dialect, and Jianghuai official dialect into the official dialect area. The CLDS data do not include the samples of Korean language region. Therefore, there are 13 dialect regions in the section of variable definition but 10 dialect areas here. [^Back]

    References

    [1] Lysgaand S. Adjustment in a Foreign Society: Norwegian Fulbright Grantees Visiting the United States. International Social Bulletin, 1955, 7: 45–51.

    [2] Gullahorn J T, Gullahorn J E. An Extension of the U-curve Hypothesis. Journal of Social Issues, 1963, 19 (3): 33–47.

    [3] Adler P. The Transnational Experience: An Alternative View of Culture Shock. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 1975, 15: 13–23.

    [4] Gordon, M M. Assimilation in American Life: The Role of Race, Religion, and National Origins. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 1964.

    [5] Berry J W, Trimble J E, Olmedo E L. Assessment of Acculturation. Cambridge Handbook of Acculturation Psychology, 1986, 8: 293–314.

    [6] Palinkas L A, Pickwell S M. Acculturation as a Risk Factor for Chronic Disease among Cambodian Refugees in the United States. Social Science & Medicine, 1995, 40 (12): 1643–1653.

    [7] Berry J W. Immigration, Acculturation, and Adaptation. Applied Psychology, 1997, 46 (1): 5–34.

    [8] Lonner W J, Berry J W. Field Methods in Cross-cultural Research. Sage Publications, 1986.

    [9] Cheung P. Acculturation and Psychiatric Morbidity among Cambodian Refugees in New Zealand. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 1995, 41 (2): 108–119.

    [10] Takeuchi D T, Chung C Y, Lin K M, et al. Lifetime and Twelve-month Prevalence Rates of Major Depressive Episodes and Dysthymia among Chinese Americans in Los Angeles. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1998, 155 (10): 1407–14.

    [11] Sonuga-Barke E J and Mistry M. The Effect of Extended Family Living on the Mental Health of Three Generations within Two Asian Communities. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 2011, 39 (2): 129–141.

    [12] Marmot M G, Syme S L. Acculturation and Coronary Heart Disease in Japanese-Americans. American Journal of Epidemiology, 1976, 104 (3): 225–247.

    [13] Reed D, McGee D, Cohen J, et al. Acculturation and Coronary Heart Disease among Japanese Men in Hawaii. American Journal of Epidemiology, 1982, 115 (6): 894–905.

    [14] Schaffer, D M, Velie E M, Shaw G M, et al. Energy and Nutrient Intakes and Health Practices of Latinas and White Non-Latinas in the 3 Months before Pregnancy. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 1998, 98: 876–884.

    [15] Abraído-lanza A F, Chao M T, Flórez K R. Do Healthy Behaviors Decline with Greater Acculturation? Implications for the Latino Mortality Paradox. Social Science & Medicine, 2005, 61 (6): 1243–1255.

    [16] Finch B K, Vega W A. Acculturation Stress, Social Support, and Self-rated Health among Latinos in California. Journal of Immigrant Health, 2003, 5 (3): 109–117.

    [17] Bzostek S, Goldman N, Pebley A. Why do Hispanics in the USA Report Poor Health? Social Science & Medicine, 2007, 65 (5): 990–1003.

    [18] Rogler L H, Cortes D E, Malgady R G. Acculturation and Mental Health Status among Hispanics. Convergence and New Directions for Research. American Psychologist, 1991, 46 (6): 585–597.

    [19] Dawson, E J, Crano, W D, Burgdoon, M. Refining the Meaning and Measurement of Acculturation: Revisiting a Novel Methodological Approach. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 1996, 20 (1): 97–114.

    [20] Kimbro R T. Acculturation in Context: Gender, Age at Migration, Neighborhood Ethnicity, and Health Behaviors. Social Science Quarterly, 2009, 90 (5): 1145–1166.

    [21] Lara M, Gamboa C, Kahramanian M I, et al. Acculturation and Latino Health in the United States: A Review of the Literature and Its Sociopolitical Context. Annual Review of Public Health, 2005, 26 (1): 367–397.

    [22] Alba R, Nee V. Rethinking Assimilation Theory for a New Era of Immigration. Int Migr Rev, 1997, 31 (4): 826–874.

    [23] Thomson M D, Hoffman-Goetz L. Defining and Measuring Acculturation: A Systematic Review of Public Health Studies with Hispanic Populations in the United States. Social Science & Medicine, 2009, 69 (7): 992–991.

    [24] Detollenaere J, Baert S, Willems S. Association between Cultural Distance and Migrant Self-rated Health. European Journal of Health Economics Hepac Health Economics in Prevention & Care, 2018, 19 (2): 257–266.

    [25] Spolaore E, Wacziarg R. Ancestry, Language and Culture. Palgrave Macmillan UK, 2016.

    [26] Babiker I E, Cox J L, Miller P M. The Measurement of Cultural Distance and Its Relationship to Medical Consultations, Symptomatology and Examination Performance of Overseas Students at Edinburgh University. Social Psychiatry, 1980, 15 (3): 109–116.

    [27] Waxin M F. Expatriates’ Interaction Adjustment: The Direct and Moderator Effects of Culture of Origin. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 2004, 28 (1): 61–79.

    [28] Qi, Y. Chinese Journal of Sociology (社会), (6): 196–215 (2014).

    [29] Kakwani N. The Relative Deprivation Curve and Its Applications. Journal of Business & Economic Statistics, 1984, 2 (4): 384–394.

    [30] Baron R M, Kenny D A. The Moderator-mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1986, 51 (6): 1173–1182.

    [31] Liu, Y., Xu, X. & Xiao, Z. Economic Research Journal (经济研究), (10): 134–146 (2015).

    [32] Benjamins M R, Hummer R A, Eberstein I W, et al. Self-reported Health and Adult Mortality Risk: An Analysis of Cause-specific Mortality. Social Science & Medicine, 2004, 59 (6): 1297–1306.

This Article

ISSN:1004-129X

CN: 22-1017/C

Vol 41, No. 01, Pages 45-57

January 2019

Downloads:0

Share
Article Outline

Abstract

  • 1 Introduction
  • 2 Literature review
  • 3 Theoretical framework and research hypotheses
  • 4 Data and variables
  • 5 Empirical analysis of the impact of cultural distance on the health status of migrants
  • 6 Conclusion
  • Footnote

    References